Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item: https://hdl.handle.net/10620/17865
Full metadata record
DC FieldValueLanguage
dc.contributor.authorHarrison, Len
dc.contributor.authorKoomen, H.M.Y.en
dc.contributor.authorSpilt, J.L.en
dc.date.accessioned2019-04-13T03:38:58Zen
dc.date.accessioned2015-06-04T01:40:44Zen
dc.date.available2015-06-04T01:40:44Zen
dc.date.issued2015-06-04en
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/10620/17865en
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10620/4136en
dc.description.abstractThere is ample research demonstrating the importance of peer-student relationships for children’s development. For teacher-student relationships as well, evidence for its influence on children’s development is accumulating. However, little is known about the relative influence of teacher-student relationships on children’s outcomes in comparison to peer-student relationships. To date, studies including both teacher-student relationships and peer-student relationships are scarce (e.g., Leflot et al., 2011; Mercer & DeRosier, 2008). There are even less, if any, studies that have examined the relative impact of teacher-student relationships and peer-student relationships on different domains of development. Considering the instructive roles of teachers, it is possible that teacher-student relationships have a larger influence on children’s cognitive development than on behavioral development in comparison to peer-student relationships. To obtain a more complete understanding of the impact of teacher-student relationships relative to peer-student relationships, the current study examined developmental associations between teacher-student relationships, peer-student relationships, conduct problems, and language competencies in a nationally-representative Australian sample. Methods. The sample included the Kindergarten cohort (N=4,983; age in months M=56.9, SD=2.6) of the ‘Longitudinal Study of Australian Children’. Teachers completed the Closeness (CL) subscale (α=.83-.85) of the Student-Teacher Relationship Scale (Pianta, 2001). Averaged scores of parent- and teacher-reports on the Peer problems scale (α=.66-.73) and the Conduct problems scale (α=.74-.75) of the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (Goodman, 1997) were used as indicators of Peer problems (PP) and Conduct problems (CP), respectively. The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised (Dunn & Dunn, 1997) measure of receptive vocabulary served as an indicator of language achievement (ACH). Measures were collected biennially at age 4/5 (Wave 1), age 6/7 (Wave 2), and age 8/9 (Wave 3). Socioeconomic status, child sex, main language spoken at home, and indigenous status were included as covariates. Autoregressive cross-lagged analyses were performed using the MLR estimator to account for non-normality. Because of the large sample size, alpha was set at .01 (two tailed) to reduce the likelihood of a Type-1 error. Parameter estimates were considered significant at the 99% confidence interval. Results. A series of nested models was tested: an autoregressive model (autoregressive paths and concurrent associations), a disorder-driven model (cross-time paths from CP and ACH to CL and PP added), a child x environment model (cross-time paths from CL and PP to CP and ACH added), and a full transactional model (cross-time paths between CL and PP added). Based on Satorra-Bentler scaled χ2difference tests for nested models (Satorra & Bentler, 2001), the child x environment model was selected as the best model (Figure 1). The fit of this model was satisfactory (χ2(20)=459.640; RMSEA=.07; SRMR=.02; CFI=.96). Conclusions. Different effects were found for peer-student and teacher-student relationships. Peer-student relationships (but not teacher-student relationships) had a significant effect on children’s behavioral development and vice versa. Importantly, teacher-student relationships (but not peer-student relationships) had a consistent and positive impact on language development. When teachers and children share a close relationship, children may be more engaged in personal conversations with the teacher, thereby advancing the child’s understanding of more sophisticated language.en
dc.subjectChild Development -- Speech and Languageen
dc.subjectChildren -- School ageen
dc.subjectChildren -- Outcomesen
dc.subjectChild Development -- Behaviouren
dc.subjectChild Development -- Cognitiveen
dc.subjectChild Development -- Socialen
dc.titleThe importance of peer-student and teacher-student relationships: Evidence from the Longitudinal Study of Australian Childrenen
dc.typeConference Papersen
dc.identifier.urlhttps://lirias.kuleuven.be/handle/123456789/479129en
dc.identifier.surveyLSACen
dc.description.keywordsbehavior problemsen
dc.description.keywordsteacher-child relationshipsen
dc.description.keywordspeer relationshipsen
dc.description.keywordsvocabularyen
dc.description.keywordscognitive developmenten
dc.description.conferencelocationSeattle, USen
dc.description.conferencenameSRCD biennial meetingen
dc.identifier.refereedYesen
local.identifier.id4649en
dc.description.formatpaper presentationen
dc.description.additionalinfohttps://lirias.kuleuven.be/handle/123456789/479129en
dc.identifier.emailjantine.spilt@ppw.kuleuven.been
dc.date.conferencestart2013en
dc.date.conferencefinish2013en
dc.date.presentation2013en
dc.subject.dssChildhood and child developmenten
dc.subject.dssmaincategoryChildrenen
dc.subject.dssmaincategoryChild Developmenten
dc.subject.dsssubcategorySchool ageen
dc.subject.dsssubcategoryOutcomesen
dc.subject.dsssubcategorySpeech and Languageen
dc.subject.dsssubcategorySocialen
dc.subject.dsssubcategoryCognitiveen
dc.subject.dsssubcategoryBehaviouren
dc.subject.flosseChildhood and child developmenten
dc.relation.surveyLSACen
dc.old.surveyvalueLSACen
item.openairecristypehttp://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_18cf-
item.cerifentitytypePublications-
item.grantfulltextnone-
item.openairetypeConference Papers-
item.fulltextNo Fulltext-
Appears in Collections:Conference Papers
Show simple item record

Page view(s)

158
checked on Dec 28, 2024
Google icon

Google ScholarTM

Check


Items in DSpace are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved, unless otherwise indicated.